34.8 Culture and consumer behavior

Markus and Kitayama (1991)

  • In Asian cultures, people prefer relatedness and interdependence, while American culture values independence (i.e., focus on the self and expressive of inner attributes)

Jennifer L. Aaker and Maheswaran (1997)

  • Prediction of dual process models is evaluated in the context of cross-culture: it’s robust across culture

    • Elaboration LIkelihood Model (Petty and Cacioppo 1979)

    • Heuristic-Systematic Model (Chaiken 1980)

    • The concurrent occurrence of both are called “additivity”. It happens when both both heuristic cue-related and attribute-related thoughts are generated (which likely under cases where one does not have contradictory info).

    • When systematic processing dominates (i.e., overriding heuristics), we call “attenuation”

    • Heuristics cues can be consensus info (which is more prevalent in collectivist culture)

  • “Perceptual differences in cue diagnosticity account for systematic differences in persuasive effects.”

  • In layman’s term, the model is transferable across cultures, but should incorporate cue diagnosticity in the model.

    • Cue diagnosticity refers to “the extent to which consumers perceive that inferences based on the information alone would be adequate to achieve their objective.” (p. 322)
  • Individualism-Collectivism (Cousins 1989; Singelis 1994; Triandis 1989)

    • Individualism: separateness, internal attributes, uniqueness of individuals

    • connectedness, social context, relationship

Rodas, John, and Torelli (2021)

  • Paradox brands (i.e., brands that have contradictory brand meanings) are more appealing to bicultural consumers (due to greater cognitive flexibility, especially those who use an acculturation strategy).

  • “a paradox brand as a brand identity that includes brand associations that appear to be contradictory in nature.” (p. 2)

    • where associations can be either specific (e.g., product benefits) or abstract/symbolic (e.g., brand personalities, value, image).
  • “brand identity” refers to “a set of brand associations that are selected by marketers to represent what the brand stands for and/or aspires to be.” (p. 2)

  • brand identity (only positive association) is from firms’ perspectives, whereas the brand image (both positive and negative association) is from consumers’ perspectives.

  • Contradiction can emerge from the unlikelihood of certain brand associations co-occurrence.

  • Brand values are defined under Schwartz (1992) (Value structure)

  • Greater cognitively flexible can lead to greater engagement in paradox brands even for monocultural consumers.

H. (Allan). Chen, Ng, and Rao (2005)

  • Cross-cultural effect on future evaluation: Westerners are more impatient thus discount the future more than Easterners. In other words, Westerners value immediate consumption more than Easterners.

  • “Easterners are faced with the threat of a delay in receiving a product (i.e., a prevention loss), they are more impatient, whereas when Westerners are faced with the threat of not being able to enjoy a product early (i.e., a promotion loss), their impatience increases.” (p. 291)

Triandis et al. (1988)

  • U.S. individualism is reflected in

    • self-reliance with competition

    • Low concern for ingroups

    • Distance from ingroups

  • While allocentric person feel that they receive more and better quality from their social support, idiocentric person feel more lonely .

Nisbett et al. (2001)

  • East Asians are more holistic with focus on assigning causality to the whole field and disregard categories and formal logic because they rely on “dialectical” reasoning

  • Westerners are more analytic because they focus on object and its category, and use formal logic.

  • “Social organization and social practices can directly affect the plausibility of metaphysical assumptions” (p. 292)

  • “Social organization and social practices can influence directly the development and use of cognitive processes” (p. 292)

References

Aaker, Jennifer L., and Durairaj Maheswaran. 1997. “The Effect of Cultural Orientation on Persuasion.” Journal of Consumer Research 24 (3): 315–28. https://doi.org/10.1086/209513.
Chaiken, Shelly. 1980. “Heuristic Versus Systematic Information Processing and the Use of Source Versus Message Cues in Persuasion.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39 (5): 752–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.5.752.
Chen, Haipeng (Allan), Sharon Ng, and Akshay R. Rao. 2005. “Cultural Differences in Consumer Impatience.” Journal of Marketing Research 42 (3): 291–301. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.2005.42.3.291.
Cousins, Steven D. 1989. “Culture and Self-Perception in Japan and the United States.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56 (1): 124–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.124.
Markus, Hazel R., and Shinobu Kitayama. 1991. “Culture and the Self: Implications for Cognition, Emotion, and Motivation.” Psychological Review 98 (2): 224–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.98.2.224.
Nisbett, Richard E., Kaiping Peng, Incheol Choi, and Ara Norenzayan. 2001. “Culture and Systems of Thought: Holistic Versus Analytic Cognition.” Psychological Review 108 (2): 291–310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.2.291.
Petty, Richard E., and John T. Cacioppo. 1979. “Issue Involvement Can Increase or Decrease Persuasion by Enhancing Message-Relevant Cognitive Responses.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37 (10): 1915–26. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1915.
Rodas, Maria A, Deborah Roedder John, and Carlos J Torelli. 2021. “Building Brands for the Emerging Bicultural Market: The Appeal of Paradox Brands.” Edited by Margaret C Campbell and Stefano Puntoni. Journal of Consumer Research, June. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucab037.
Schwartz, Shalom H. 1992. “Universals in the Content and Structure of Values: Theoretical Advances and Empirical Tests in 20 Countries.” In, 1–65. Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60281-6.
Singelis, Theodore M. 1994. “The Measurement of Independent and Interdependent Self-Construals.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 20 (5): 580–91. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205014.
Triandis, Harry C. 1989. “The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cultural Contexts.” Psychological Review 96 (3): 506–20. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.96.3.506.
Triandis, Harry C., Robert Bontempo, Marcelo J. Villareal, Masaaki Asai, and Nydia Lucca. 1988. “Individualism and Collectivism: Cross-Cultural Perspectives on Self-Ingroup Relationships.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54 (2): 323–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.2.323.