Quality of graphs

Sources: Original material; Healy (2018); Tufte (2001);

1 Are there general quality criteria?

  • “Visualization is surprisingly difficult. Even the most simple matters can easily go wrong” (Cleveland 1994, 12)
  • Central question: Are there general criteria that we can use to evaluate graphs?
  • Criteria depend on various aspects… (see Section 11)
    • Audience: For what kind of audience do make your graph? For yourself? Listeners of your talk? Readers of your paper? What background do they have and can they understand the graph (Clarity)?
      • Important: Whatever general principles there are, your audience (and you) is the final judge!
    • Aim: What goal do you want to achieve with your graphics?
      • e.g., explore data, explain data, persuade/convince audience of a fact


2 Aims of visualizing

  • Exploratory graphs: “forces us to notice what we never expected to see” (Tukey 1977, vi)
    • Audience: Ourselves (during research process)
    • Objective(s): Explore and interact with the data (size, complexity, characteristics, distribution)
    • Approach: Produce many graphs (quick and dirty)
    • Interactivity is great for exploration!
  • Explanatory graphs (presentation): “forces readers to see the information the designer wanted to convey” (Stephen Michael Kosslyn 1994, 271)
    • Audience: Others (of different characteristics) at the end of our research
    • Objective(s): Show to, communicate to and convince others (tell story)
    • Approach: Produce a final graph that is carefully crafted (and beautiful)
  • Convergence/overlap between strictly exploratory and confirmatory appraoches (Healy and Moody 2014, 113)
  • Insight: What is a “good” graph depends on our aim & audience!

3 What makes bad figures bad?

Based on Healy (2018, ch. 1.2).

3.1 Bad taste & chart junk

Figure 1: Source (Healy 2018, Ch 1, Figure 1.4: A chart with a considerable amount of junk in it): https://socviz.co/lookatdata.html
Insights
  • Graph illustrates various bad choices
    • x-axis: Labelling (0, 22.5, etc.) is unintuitive and hard to grasp
    • y-axis: Countries are ordered alphabetically (more difficult decode ranking of contintents)
    • Mapping: Continent is mapped both to color and y-axis (only use one mapping per variable)
    • Font: Harder to read and partly capitalized (“CONTINENT”)
    • Clutter & jung: Background & shades are unnecessary (too much ink!)
    • 3D: Makes it harder to decode the graph, i.e., extract the numerical values that are visualized graph
    • Complexity: Graph visualizes 5 numbers.. questionable whether we would need graph for that.
    • Notes: Absence of figure notes makes it hard to understand what is shown (e.g., how is life expectancy measured?)



Figure 2: Source (Healy 2018, Ch 1, Figure 1.6: ‘Monstrous Costs’ by Nigel Holmes. Also a classic of its kind.): https://socviz.co/lookatdata.html
Insights
  • Sometimes illustrators may choose memorability/story over interpretability

3.2 Bad data!

  • “Well-designed figures with little or no junk in their component parts are not by themselves a defence against cherry-picking your data, or presenting information in a misleading way.” (Healy 2018, Ch. 1.2.2)

Figure 3 and Figure 4 illustrate the relevance of the underlying data.

Figure 3: Source (Healy 2018, Ch 1, Figure 1.8: A crisis of faith in democracy? (New York Times.)): https://socviz.co/lookatdata.html



Figure 4: Source (Healy 2018, Ch 1, Figure 1.9: Perhaps the crisis has been overblown. (Erik Voeten.)): https://socviz.co/lookatdata.html
  • “Rather, a different measure is being shown. We are now looking at the trend in the average score, rather than the trend for the highest possible answer. Substantively, there does still seem to be a decline in the average score by age cohort, on the order of between half a point to one and a half points on a ten point scale.” (Healy 2018, 9–10)

3.3 Bad perception!

  • “Visualizations encode numbers in lines, shapes, and colors. That means that our interpretation of these encodings is partly conditional on how we perceive geometric shapes and relationships generally. We have known for a long time that poorly-encoded data can be misleading.” (Healy 2018, Ch. 1.2.3)

  • Please inpsect Figure 5, Figure 6 and Figure 7: Why are those graphs bad/good/striking?

Figure 5: Source (Healy 2018, Ch 1, Figure 1.10: A 3-D Column Chart created in Microsoft Excel for Mac. Although it may seem hard to believe, the values shown in the bars are 1, 2, 3, and 4.): https://socviz.co/lookatdata.html



Figure 6: Source (Healy 2018, Ch 1, Figure 1.11: A junk-free plot that remains hard to interpret. While a stacked bar chart makes the overall trend clear, it can make it harder to see the trends for the categories within the bar. This is partly due to the nature of the trends. But if the additional data is hard to understand, perhaps it should not be included to begin with.): https://socviz.co/lookatdata.html



Figure 7: Source (Healy 2018, Ch 1, Figure 1.12: Aspect ratios affect our perception of rates of change. (After an example by William S. Cleveland.)): https://socviz.co/lookatdata.html

3.4 Dataviz caveats

  • Beyond considering bad taste/chart junk, bad data and bad perception we can also follow common data viz caveats
  • The collection by data-to-viz.com provides a good overview
  • A few examples
    • Alphabetical ordering: Categorical variables often displayed in alphabetical ordering (“Alabama first!” mistake (Wainer 2005)) (e.g., here)
    • Overplotting: Too many points may make us fail to see density (e.g., here or here)
    • Mappings vs. variables, e.g., country is encoded both on y-axis and through color but only one of two mappings would be necessary
  • Homework: Read through the collection of dataviz caveats.. which are the most important ones?

4 Tufte: Graphical excellence

4.1 Edward Tufte

  • Quotes from his website.
    • “One visionary day….the insights of this class lead to new levels of understanding both for creators and viewers of visual displays.” WIRED
    • “The Leonardo da Vinci of data.” THE NEW YORK TIMES
    • “The Galileo of graphics.” BLOOMBERG
  • Tufte provides recommendations/insights across his books
  • We’ll mostly focus on “The Visual Display of Quantitative Information” (Tufte 2001)

4.2 Graphical displays should…

  • “Excellence in statistical graphics consists of complex ideas communicated with clarity, precision and efficiency” (Tufte 2001, 2:13)
  • “Graphics reveal data. Indeed graphics can be more precise and revealing than conventional statistical computations.” (Tufte 2001, 2:13)
  • “Graphical displays should:
    • show the data
    • induce the viewer to think about the substance rather than about methodology, graphic design, the technology of graphic production or something else
    • avoid distorting what the data has to say
    • present many numbers in a small space
    • make large data sets coherent
    • encourage the eye to compare different pieces of data
    • reveal the data at several levels of detail, from a broad overview to the fine structure
    • serve a reasonably clear purpose: description, exploration, tabulation or decoration
    • be closely integrated with the statistical and verbal descriptions of a data set.” (Tufte 2001, 2:13)
  • Remember the “best graph” ever drawn.

4.3 Principals of graphical excellence

  • Graphical excellence… (Tufte 2001, vol. 2, chaps. 1, 51)
    • …is the well-designed presentation of interesting data - a matter of substance, of statistics, and of design.
    • …consists of complex ideas communicated with clarity, precision, and efficiency.
    • …is that which gives to the viewer the greatest number of ideas in the shortest time with the least ink in the smallest space.
    • …is nearly always multivariate.
    • …requires telling the truth about the data.
  • Q: Do our plots reflect these ideas?

5 Tufte: Graphical integrity (Skip!)

5.1 Graphical integrity (1)

  • Principles of graphical integrity (Tufte 2001, vol. 2, chaps. 2, 77)
    • The representation of numbers, as physically measured on the surface of the graphic itself, should be directly proportional to the numerical quantities represented.
    • Clear, detailed, and thorough labeling should be used to defeat graphical distortion and ambiguity. Write out explanations of the data on the graphic itself. Label important events in the data.
    • Show data variation, not design variation.
    • In time-series displays of money, deflated and standardized units of monetary measurement are nearly always better than nominal units.
    • The number of information-carrying (variable) dimensions depicted should not exceed the number of dimensions in the data.
    • Graphics must not quote data out of context.

5.2 Graphical integrity (2): Example & exercise

Figure 8: Global deaths


Figure 9: Daily deaths


Figure 10: New cases

5.3 Graphical integrity (3): Example & exercise

Figure 11: Global deaths
    1. “allowed the government to say that, even after adding non-hospital deaths in England to its daily tally, the UK’s rate was below Belgium, Spain and Italy and almost in step with France” (McIntyre and Duncan 2020)
    • “the small print under the graphic proved important:”Differences between countries’ trajectories can reflect differences in determining cause of death, testing capacity, and interventions (eg social distancing measures) implemented.” (McIntyre and Duncan 2020)
    • The devil was in the detail: the UK’s daily figures only included deaths where the patient tested positive for Covid-19. The Belgian figures included all suspected cases regardless of whether a test was carried out.
  • Chart by Guaridan below:
Figure 12: Global deaths
  • “if we strip out nursing home deaths in the UK and Belgian, things look a lot different.” (McIntyre and Duncan 2020)
  • “A total of 3,419 hospital deaths had been recorded in Belgium by 29 April, or 295 deaths per 1 million population. There were 22,286 hospital deaths in the UK recorded by the same day (excluding those who did not receive a positive test beforehand) or 328 deaths per 1 million population.”; “The deaths each government includes or excludes matter. France includes care home deaths in its total, whereas the UK only includes some care-home deaths as set out above.” (McIntyre and Duncan 2020)



Figure 13: Daily deaths
  • “The daily death toll figures released by the government are not an accurate reflection of the number of deaths in the UK”
    • “Up to 28 April, the figures included only those hospital deaths where a patient had received a positive Covid-19 test. Since 29 April, the figures have included non-hospital deaths in England. Deaths in other settings in the devolved nations were already being counted in the daily government figure.” (McIntyre and Duncan 2020)
    • “However, the requirement for a positive test still stands, meaning hundreds of deaths are missing from the daily toll.” (McIntyre and Duncan 2020)
    • confusion as to whether the government figures are per date of reported death or date of actual death – the figures fluctuate day by day which could be due to corrections to the daily totals, making it hard to be sure.” (McIntyre and Duncan 2020)
    • “regardless of which metrics are being used, the figures in the bar chart on the government’s screen each day – including its use of a seven-day rolling average – is still not a fair reflection of the current situation.” (McIntyre and Duncan 2020)



Figure 14: New cases
  • “The briefing also contains an update on the number of new coronavirus cases confirmed each day. During the 4 May press conference 3,985 new cases were confirmed by lab testing. Despite the nice bar graph, we have no idea how many cases there are in the UK.” (McIntyre and Duncan 2020)
  • “Although testing capacity has increased substantially, the UK is nowhere near the widespread”test and trace” strategy recommended by the World Health Organization and followed by other countries such as Germany and South Korea.” (McIntyre and Duncan 2020)

Another recent example (not sure if it’s true…).

  • Questions we should always ask…
    • How are these things measures (are measures valid)? Was every of the underlying data points measured in the same way?
    • What is the time period? Does time correspond to measurement?

6 Tufte: Theory of data graphics

  • Large share of ink on a graphic should present data-information, the ink changing as the data change (Tufte 2001, 2:93)
  • Data-ink: Non-erasable core of a graphic, the non-redundant ink arranged in response to variation in the numbers represented (Tufte 2001, 2:93)


  • \(\text{Data-ink ratio} = \frac{\text{data-ink}}{\text{total ink used to print the graphic}}\) = proportion of a graphic’s ink devoted to the non-redundant display of data-information = 1.0 - proportion of a graphic that can be erased without loss of data-information (Tufte 2001, 2:93)


  • Rule: Maximize the data-ink ratio, within reason (Tufte 2001, 2:96)
  • Two Erasing Principles (Tufte 2001, 2:96)
    • Erase non-data-ink, within reason
    • Erase redundant data-ink, within reason

6.1 Data-ink Exercise 1

  • Q: How could we change Figure 15) with the aim of erasing non-data-ink (within reason) and erasing redundant data-ink (within reason)?
Figure 15: Data-ink

6.2 Data-ink Exercise 1 (Solution)

  • Tufte provides examples in his book and Tufte in R provides R code
  • See Figure 16) below
    • Q: What is shown? Which ones use less/more data? Which ones do you prefer? (Principles vs. readability)1 2
Figure 16: Reducing data-ink: Example 1

6.3 Data-ink Exercise 2

  • Q: How could we change Figure 17) (Bauer and Poama 2020) with the aim of erasing non-data-ink (within reason) and erasing redundant data-ink (within reason)?
Figure 17: Data-ink

6.4 Data-ink Exercise 2 (Solution)

  • Q: See Figure 18) below: What is shown? Which ones use less/more data-ink? Which ones do you prefer?3
Figure 18: Reducing data-ink: Example 2

6.5 Principles

  • 5 principles produce substantial changes in graphical design. The principles apply to many graphics and yield a series of design options through cycles of graphical revision and editing. (Tufte 2001, 2:105)
  1. Above all else show the data.
  2. Maximize the data-ink ratio.
  3. Erase non-data-ink.
  4. Erase redundant data-ink.
  5. Revise and edit.

6.6 Data density and small multiples (1)


  • \(\text{Data density of a graphic} = \frac{\text{number of entries in data matrix}}{\text{area of data graphic}}\) (Tufte 2001, 2:168)

  • Further rules:

    • Maximize data density and the size of the data matrix, within reason
    • Graphics can be shrunk way down.
    • → small multiples
  • Figure 19, a small-multiples example, was published in Bauer and Morisi (2023).

    • Q: How many variables are visualized and on what kind of scales?
Figure 19: Means and variance of trust over time


  • Figure 20, a small-multiples example, was published in Munzert and Bauer (2013).4
Figure 20: Polarization trends


  • Figure 21 uses a heatmap to display trends (negative or positive).
Figure 21: Polarization trends

6.7 Data density and small multiples (2)

  • Tufte (2001, 2:175): “Well-designed small multiples are
    • inevitably comparative
    • deftly multivariate
    • shrunken, high-density graphics
    • usually based on a large data matrix
    • drawn almost entirely with data-ink
    • efficient in interpretation
    • often narrative in content, showing shifts in the relationship between variables as the index variable changes (thereby revealing interaction or multiplicative effects ).”
  • Small multiples reflect much of the theory of data graphics:

6.8 Aesthetics and Technique in Graphical Design (1)

  • Discusses different issues: Choice of design (text vs. table); Combining words & numbers; Proportion & Scale (Line weight and lettering), Shape of graphics (2 \(\times\) 1)5
  • Graphical elegance is often found in simplicity of design and complexity of data (Tufte 2001, 2:177)
  • “Attractive displays of statistical information
    • have a properly chosen format and design
    • use words, numbers, and drawing together
    • reflect a balance, a proportion, a sense of relevant scale
    • display an accessible complexity of detail
    • often have a narrative quality, a story to tell about the data
    • are drawn in a professional manner, with the technical details of production done with care
    • avoid content-free decoration, including chartjunk.” (Tufte 2001, 2:177)
  • Examples: Tufte (2001) (Table page 179, Figure 180),

6.9 Aesthetics and Technique in Graphical Design (2)

  • Below Tufte (2001, 2:183)’s criteria for a friendly data graphic, i.e., recommendations to make complex things accessible (accessible complexity): We don’t have to agree with everything!
Friendly Unfriendly
words are spelled out, mysterious and elaborate encoding avoided abbreviations abound, requiring the viewer to sort through text to decode abbreviations
words run from left to right, the usual direction for reading occidental languages words run vertically, particularly along the Y-axis; words run in several different directions
little messages help explain data graphic is cryptic, requires repeated references to scattered text
elaborately encoded shadings, cross-hatching, and colors are avoided; instead, labels are placed on the graphic itself; no legend is required obscure codings require going back and forth between legend and graphic
graphic attracts viewer, provokes curiosity graphic is repellent, filled with chartjunk
colors, if used, are chosen so that the color-deficient and color-blind (5 to 10 percent of viewers) can make sense of the graphic (blue can be distinguished from other colors by most color-deficient people) design insensitive to color-deficient viewers; red and green used for essential contrasts
type is clear, precise, modest; lettering may be done by hand type is clotted, overbearing
type is upper-and-lower case, with serifs type is all capitals, sans serif

6.10 Design is choice

Design is choice
  • The theory of the visual display of quantitative information = principles that generate design options and that guide choices among options
  • Principles should not be applied rigidly
  • Better to violate any principle than to place graceless or inelegant marks on paper
  • Greet principles with skepticism
  • Search for clear portrayal of complexity. Not the complication of the simple
The revelation of the complex

7 Perception & decoding

7.1 Perception & decoding (1)

  • “an effective display must be easily encoded and comprehended by the visual information-processing system” (Stephen M. Kosslyn 1989, 192)
  • Decoding, i.e., perception and interpretation involve specific visual tasks
    • Data values were encoded or mapped in to the graph, and now we have to get them back out again
      • Test: Ask questions about your graph.. what value does Europe have? Bigger or smaller than Americas? (cf. Figure 1)
  • Decoding edges & colors (color blindness, pick the right ones: colorbrewer, HCL, localization/cultures6)
  • Preattentive search & patterns (see Figure 1.18 in Healy (2018) and Section 8)

7.2 Perception & decoding (2)

  • Very good discussion in Healy (2018), Chapter 1.3 and 1.4
  • “For each graph type, subjects were asked to identify the smaller of two marked segments on the chart, and then to”make a quick visual judgment” estimating what percentage the smaller one was of the larger. As can be seen from the figure, the charts tested encoded data in different ways.” (Source)7

Source (Healy 2018, Ch 1, Figure 1.22: Schematic representation of basic perceptual tasks for nine chart types, by Heer and Bostock, following Cleveland and McGill. In both studies, participants were asked to make comparisons of highlighted portions of each chart type, and say which was smaller.): https://socviz.co/lookatdata.html

Source (Healy 2018, Ch 1, Figure 1.23: Cleveland and McGill’s original results (top) and Heer and Bostock’s replication with additions (bottom) for nine chart types.): https://socviz.co/lookatdata.html
  • “The overall pattern of results seems clear, with performance worsening substantially as we move away from comparison on a common scale to length-based comparisons to angles and finally areas. Area comparisons perform even worse than the (justifiably) much-maligned pie chart.” (Healy 2018, 24–25)

8 Focusing attention

Based on Nussbaumer Knaflic (2015, Ch. 4).

8.1 Memory

  • Three types of memory are important
  • Iconic memory (= visual sensory memory): pertains to the visual domain and a fast-decaying store of visual information8
    • Processes visual information incredibly quickly, influenced by evolutionary adaptations for survival
    • Information briefly resides in iconic memory before being transferred to short-term memory, with preattentive attributes being crucial for effective visual communication and design.
  • Short-term memory: capacity for holding a small amount of information in an active, readily available state for a short interval
  • Long-term memory: long-term storage
    • e.g., images can help us more quickly recall things stored in our long‐term verbal memory (e.g., picture of Eiffel Tower)
  • Strategies for focusing attention focus on the iconic memory

8.2 Preattentive attributes signal where to look

  • Try to search for the blue dot and start with the image on the left in Figure 22.
Figure 22: Searching for the blue circle becomes progressively harder (Healy 2018, Ch 1.3.2, Figure 1.18)


  • Nussbaumer Knaflic (2015) uses an example like Figure 23 to illustrate the power of preattentive attributes. Please try to count the number of 2s.
Now with a bit of color…
  • Now try to do the same exercise again in Figure 24. Has it become easier?
Figure 24: Count the 2s

8.3 Preattentive attributes

  • Figure 25 by Nussbaumer Knaflic (2015) illustrates preattentive attributes: What are your eyes drawn to (our brains are hardwired to do so)?
Figure 25: Preattentive attributes (Nussbaumer Knaflic 2015, Figure 4.4)
  • Nussbaumer Knaflic (2015) also discusses preattentive attributes in text (make sure to use them!)
    • Check out Fig 4.5 in her book.

8.4 Using preattentive attributes in graphs

  • See Nussbaumer Knaflic (2015), Fig. 4.7, 4.8 and 4.9 for examples of highlighting facts
  • Strategy: Push everything in the background → make the data stand out → add marks for data points/numeric labels → reduce markers to the essential
    • See Fig. 4.10 - 4.14 for an examples of this strategy
  • Warning: When you highlight one aspect it can make other things harder to see (Nussbaumer Knaflic 2015, 112)
  • Ideally: Give the graph to someone else and ask…
    • …what stands out?
    • …what main message he/she reads?

9 Retake: What did discuss?

Q: What do you remember?



  • What makes figures bad?
    • Bad taste, chart junk, bad data, bad perception, caveats
  • Edward Tufte
    • Graphical integrity
    • Theory of data graphics (Tufte): Data-ink, data density friendly/unfriendly aesthetics
  • Decoding & human perception
  • Focusing attention

10 Can software save us?

  • Can software (such as ggplot2) solve those problems?
    • ggplot2 = spellchecker: Plot won’t work unless components correctly specified (Wickham 2010, 24–25)
    • ggplot2 \(\neq\) grammar checker: Would need to identify common mistakes/warn user (Wickham 2010, 24–25)
    • Tools: Default template, default parameters.. but mostly we need education! (Wickham 2010, 25)
  • Q: What about AI (e.g., GPT-4)?
    • Social media lighting up… (e.g., a few examples on Twitter)
    • First we would need to train them…

11 Good Graph Checklist

  • Audience: Set of individuals for whom the graph was made
    • Vary in terms of attributes (e.g., age, discipline) and mode/display through which they consume your graph (e.g., us, audience of presentation, readers of book)
  • General questions
    1. Relevance: Does the graph answer a (our) question?
    2. Clarity: Does the audience understand our graph?
    3. Effectiveness: Does the graph achieve our aim (e.g., convincing an audience of a fact)?
  • More fine-grained questions
    1. Legibility: Can the audience read/decipher/decode everything depending on display (e.g., presentation slides, website, book, paper)?
    2. Familiarity: Has the audience encountered this type of graph before?
    3. Accessibility: How long do we need to understand what’s being shown?
    4. Completeness: Do we have all the information we need to understand it?
    5. Aesthetics: Is the graph visually pleasing?
    6. Narrative: Does the graph tell a story?
    7. Simplicity & complexity: Does the graph both provide a simple message but also allow to dive into the details of the data?

References

Bauer, Paul C, and Davide Morisi. 2023. “Has Trust in the European Parliament Polarized?” Socius 9 (January): 23780231231175430.
Bauer, Paul C, and Andrei Poama. 2020. “Does Suffering Suffice? An Experimental Assessment of Desert Retributivism.” PLoS One 15 (4): e0230304.
Cleveland, W S. 1994. “The Elements of Graphing Data.” NJ: Hobart Press.
Healy, Kieran. 2018. Data Visualization: A Practical Introduction. Princeton University Press.
Healy, Kieran, and James Moody. 2014. “Data Visualization in Sociology.” Annu. Rev. Sociol. 40 (July): 105–28.
Kosslyn, Stephen M. 1989. “Understanding Charts and Graphs.” Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 3 (3): 185–225.
Kosslyn, Stephen Michael. 1994. Elements of Graph Design. WH Freeman.
McIntyre, Niamh, and Pamela Duncan. 2020. “Why No 10’s Covid-19 Death Toll Slides Don’t Tell the Whole Story.” The Guardian, May.
Munzert, Simon, and Paul C Bauer. 2013. “Political Depolarization in German Public Opinion, 1980–2010.” Political Science Research and Methods 1 (1): 67–89.
Nussbaumer Knaflic, Cole. 2015. Storytelling with Data: A Data Visualization Guide for Business Professionals. John Wiley & Sons.
Tufte, Edward R. 2001. The Visual Display of Quantitative Information. Vol. 2. Graphics press Cheshire, CT.
Tukey, John W. 1977. Exploratory Data Analysis. Vol. 2. Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley Publishing.
Wainer, Howard. 2005. Graphic Discovery: A Trout in the Milk and Other Visual Adventures. Princeton University Press.
Wickham, Hadley. 2010. “A Layered Grammar of Graphics.” J. Comput. Graph. Stat. 19 (1): 3–28.
———. 2016. Ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer.

Footnotes

  1. See Tufte (2001, 2:123ff) for analogue graphs.↩︎

  2. “The grey background gives the plot a sim-ilar typographic colour to the text, ensuring that the graphics fit in with theflow of a document without jumping out with a bright white background. Finally, the grey background creates a continuous field of colour which ensures that the plot is perceived as a single visual entity” (Wickham 2016, 172).↩︎

  3. See Tufte (2001, 2:123ff) for analogue graphs.↩︎

  4. Figure 4 provides an overview of the estimated trends. All figures visualize results from multilevel models based on the same logic as the models presented above. Both intercepts and slopes are allowed to vary for each sub-group. Models visualized under the label ‘Overall’ display the average trend across all attitude scales and are specified in Model A. The four other panels under each sub-group distinguish between issue dimensions-that is, they report results from Model C specification. While each of the figures may tell its own complex story—and should be the subject of greater study in the future—our focus is on general patterns. Levels and trends differ considerably between certain sub-groups, but not between all. We want to point out the findings we regard as the most illuminating. Regarding the overall trend (Column 1), there are no significant differences for sub- populations of gender, income and religious groups. However, the overall decreasing trend is much stronger among highly educated, highly interested people, and somewhat stronger among respondents from West Germany. The trend of increasing polarization on the gender dimension (Column 2) is similar across most sub-populations. Only the difference between the more and less educated is striking. Whereas the more educated display no increase (or a decrease) in attitude alignment, the increase in polarization seems to take place among the less educated on this dimension. Concerning the moral dimension (Column 3), the plots reveal a general decreasing trend of polarization across several sub-populations. On the distribution dimension (Column 4) , we again find significant differences between sub-populations of education, political interest and respondents from West and East Germany. In contrast, trends are similar among the other sub-populations. On the immigration dimension (Column 5) , trends are similar across most sub-populations .↩︎

  5. “Visual preferences for rectangular proportions have been studied by psychologists since 1860, but, even given the implausible assumption that such studies are relevant to graphic design, the findings are hardly decisive. A mild preference for proportions near to the Golden Rectangle is found among those taking part in the experiments, but the preferred height/length ratios also vary a great deal” (Tufte 2001, 2:190).↩︎

  6. Colors may have different effects and meanings across cultures.↩︎

  7. “Types 1-3 use position encoding along a common scale while Type 4 and 5 use length encoding. The pie chart encodes values as angles, and the remaining charts as areas, either using circular, separate rectangles (as in a cartogram) or sub-rectangles (as in a treemap).”↩︎

  8. “The mental representation of the visual stimuli are referred to as icons (fleeting images.) Iconic memory was the first sensory store to be investigated with experiments dating back as far as 1740. One of the earliest investigations into this phenomenon was by Ján Andrej Segner, a German physicist and mathematician. In his experiment, Segner attached a glowing coal to a cart wheel and rotated the wheel at increasing speed until an unbroken circle of light was perceived by the observer. He calculated that the glowing coal needed to make a complete circle in under 100ms to achieve this effect, which he determined was the duration of this visual memory store. In 1960, George Sperling conducted a study where participants were shown a set of letters for a brief amount of time and were asked to recall the letters they were shown afterwards. Participants were less likely to recall more letters when asked about the whole group of letters, but recalled more when asked about specific subgroups of the whole. These findings suggest that iconic memory in humans has a large capacity, but decays very rapidly.” (Wikipedia)↩︎