4 Causal Steps, Confounding, and Causal Order

Comfort with [the principles of the basic mediation model] allows you to conduct mediation analysis and use it to shed light on your research questions and hypotheses about causal processes. In this chapter, [we] take up a variety of complications, including testing and ruling out various alternative explanations for associations observed in a mediation analysis, effect size, and models with multiple causal agents and outcomes. (p. 113)

4.1 What about Barron and Kenny?

Complete and partial mediation are concepts that are deeply ingrained in the thinking of social and behavioral scientists. But I just don’t see what they offer our understanding of a phenomenon. They are too sample-size-dependent and the distinction between them has no substantive or theoretical meaning or value of any consequence. I recommend avoiding expressing hypotheses about mediation or results of a mediation analysis using these terms. (p. 121)

Agreed.

4.2 Confounding and causal order

One of the beautiful features of experiments is the causal interpretations they afford about differences between groups. Good experimentation is tough and requires lots of careful planning and strict control over experimental procedures, construction of stimuli, treatment of participants, and so forth. But when done well, no research design gives a researcher more confidence in the claim that differences between groups defined by \(X\) on some variable of interest is due to \(X\) rather than something else. Given that a mediation model is a causal model, the ability to make unequivocal causal claims about the effect of \(X\) on \(M\) and the direct and total effects of \(X\) on \(Y\) gives experiments tremendous appeal.

Absent random assignment to values of \(X\), all of the associations in a mediation model are susceptible to confounding and epiphenomenal association, not just the association between \(M\) and \(Y\). Whether one’s design includes manipulation and random assignment of \(X\) or not, it behooves the researcher to seriously ponder these potential threats to causal inference and, if possible, do something to reduce their plausibility as alternative explanations for associations observed. (pp 121–122, emphasis in the original)

4.2.1 Accounting for confounding and epiphenomenal association.

Here we load a couple necessary packages, load the data, and take a peek at them.

## Observations: 262
## Variables: 7
## $ tenure   <dbl> 1.67, 0.58, 0.58, 2.00, 5.00, 9.00, 0.00, 2.50, 0.50, 0.58, 9.00, 1.92, 2.00, 1.42, 0.92, …
## $ estress  <dbl> 6.0, 5.0, 5.5, 3.0, 4.5, 6.0, 5.5, 3.0, 5.5, 6.0, 5.5, 4.0, 3.0, 2.5, 3.5, 6.0, 4.0, 6.0, …
## $ affect   <dbl> 2.60, 1.00, 2.40, 1.16, 1.00, 1.50, 1.00, 1.16, 1.33, 3.00, 3.00, 2.00, 1.83, 1.16, 1.16, …
## $ withdraw <dbl> 3.00, 1.00, 3.66, 4.66, 4.33, 3.00, 1.00, 1.00, 2.00, 4.00, 4.33, 1.00, 5.00, 1.66, 4.00, …
## $ sex      <dbl> 1, 0, 1, 1, 1, 1, 0, 0, 1, 1, 1, 1, 1, 1, 1, 1, 1, 1, 0, 0, 0, 1, 1, 1, 0, 1, 0, 0, 0, 1, …
## $ age      <dbl> 51, 45, 42, 50, 48, 48, 51, 47, 40, 43, 57, 36, 33, 29, 33, 48, 40, 45, 37, 42, 54, 57, 37…
## $ ese      <dbl> 5.33, 6.05, 5.26, 4.35, 4.86, 5.05, 3.66, 6.13, 5.26, 4.00, 2.53, 6.60, 5.20, 5.66, 5.66, …

The lowerCor() function from the psych package makes it easy to estimate the lower triangle of a correlation matrix.

##          tenure estrss affect wthdrw sex    age    ese   
## tenure    1.000                                          
## estress   0.068  1.000                                   
## affect   -0.065  0.340  1.000                            
## withdraw -0.035  0.064  0.417  1.000                     
## sex      -0.003  0.133  0.046  0.050  1.000              
## age       0.266  0.066 -0.018 -0.035  0.083  1.000       
## ese      -0.060 -0.158 -0.246 -0.243  0.028 -0.083  1.000

Let’s open brms.

Recall that if you want the correlations with Bayesian estimation and those sweet Bayesian credible intervals, you set up an intercept-only multivariate model.

Behold the summary.

##  Family: MV(gaussian, gaussian, gaussian, gaussian) 
##   Links: mu = identity; sigma = identity
##          mu = identity; sigma = identity
##          mu = identity; sigma = identity
##          mu = identity; sigma = identity 
## Formula: ese ~ 1 
##          estress ~ 1 
##          affect ~ 1 
##          withdraw ~ 1 
##    Data: estress (Number of observations: 262) 
## Samples: 4 chains, each with iter = 2000; warmup = 1000; thin = 1;
##          total post-warmup samples = 4000
## 
## Population-Level Effects: 
##                    Estimate Est.Error l-95% CI u-95% CI  Rhat Bulk_ESS Tail_ESS
## ese_Intercept         5.607     0.062    5.487    5.731 1.002     6703     2700
## estress_Intercept     4.621     0.090    4.451    4.798 1.000     7513     3385
## affect_Intercept      1.598     0.045    1.508    1.685 1.000     4887     3304
## withdraw_Intercept    2.321     0.078    2.166    2.471 1.000     4763     3198
## 
## Family Specific Parameters: 
##                Estimate Est.Error l-95% CI u-95% CI  Rhat Bulk_ESS Tail_ESS
## sigma_ese         0.954     0.041    0.875    1.039 1.000     6984     3141
## sigma_estress     1.437     0.062    1.320    1.563 1.002     6673     3338
## sigma_affect      0.729     0.032    0.673    0.795 1.000     5250     3334
## sigma_withdraw    1.257     0.055    1.157    1.368 1.001     6613     3392
## 
## Residual Correlations: 
##                          Estimate Est.Error l-95% CI u-95% CI  Rhat Bulk_ESS Tail_ESS
## rescor(ese,estress)        -0.155     0.060   -0.270   -0.034 1.001     7076     2855
## rescor(ese,affect)         -0.240     0.059   -0.352   -0.121 1.002     5117     3290
## rescor(estress,affect)      0.334     0.056    0.222    0.441 1.001     4837     2871
## rescor(ese,withdraw)       -0.237     0.059   -0.350   -0.116 1.000     6195     3246
## rescor(estress,withdraw)    0.059     0.062   -0.064    0.177 1.000     4775     3077
## rescor(affect,withdraw)     0.410     0.051    0.307    0.505 1.000     5701     3463
## 
## Samples were drawn using sampling(NUTS). For each parameter, Eff.Sample 
## is a crude measure of effective sample size, and Rhat is the potential 
## scale reduction factor on split chains (at convergence, Rhat = 1).

Since we have posteriors for the correlations, why not plot them? Here we use the theme_black() from brms and a color scheme from the viridis package.

In the last chapter, we said there were multiple ways to set up a multivariate model in brms. Our first approach was to externally define the submodels using the bf() function, save them as objects, and then include those objects within the brm() function. Another approach is to just define the separate bf() submodels directly in the brm() function, combining them with the + operator. That’s the approach we will practice in this chapter. Here’s what it looks like for our first mediation model.

Worked like a charm. Here’s the summary.

##  Family: MV(gaussian, gaussian) 
##   Links: mu = identity; sigma = identity
##          mu = identity; sigma = identity 
## Formula: withdraw ~ 1 + estress + affect + ese + sex + tenure 
##          affect ~ 1 + estress + ese + sex + tenure 
##    Data: estress (Number of observations: 262) 
## Samples: 4 chains, each with iter = 2000; warmup = 1000; thin = 1;
##          total post-warmup samples = 4000
## 
## Population-Level Effects: 
##                    Estimate Est.Error l-95% CI u-95% CI  Rhat Bulk_ESS Tail_ESS
## withdraw_Intercept    2.735     0.536    1.689    3.788 1.002     8475     2689
## affect_Intercept      1.783     0.308    1.184    2.388 1.000     8891     3210
## withdraw_estress     -0.093     0.053   -0.195    0.013 1.002     7910     2629
## withdraw_affect       0.708     0.104    0.506    0.913 1.000     6871     3244
## withdraw_ese         -0.211     0.075   -0.363   -0.062 1.002     8788     3017
## withdraw_sex          0.125     0.142   -0.154    0.402 1.001     7881     3132
## withdraw_tenure      -0.002     0.011   -0.022    0.019 1.000     8692     3171
## affect_estress        0.159     0.030    0.099    0.219 1.001     8703     3026
## affect_ese           -0.154     0.044   -0.240   -0.071 1.000     8025     3123
## affect_sex            0.014     0.086   -0.153    0.183 1.000     9290     2854
## affect_tenure        -0.011     0.006   -0.023    0.001 1.000    10720     3474
## 
## Family Specific Parameters: 
##                Estimate Est.Error l-95% CI u-95% CI  Rhat Bulk_ESS Tail_ESS
## sigma_withdraw    1.127     0.051    1.032    1.231 1.001     8047     2943
## sigma_affect      0.671     0.029    0.617    0.732 1.000     7653     3026
## 
## Samples were drawn using sampling(NUTS). For each parameter, Eff.Sample 
## is a crude measure of effective sample size, and Rhat is the potential 
## scale reduction factor on split chains (at convergence, Rhat = 1).

In the printout, notice how first within intercepts and then with covariates and sigma, the coefficients are presented as for withdraw first and then affect. Also notice how the coefficients for the covariates are presented in the same order for each criterions. Hopefully that’ll make it easier to sift through the printout. Happily, our coefficients are quite similar to those in Table 4.1.

Here are the \(R^2\) summaries.

##            Estimate Est.Error  Q2.5 Q97.5
## R2withdraw    0.213     0.038 0.139 0.287
## R2affect      0.170     0.037 0.099 0.242

These are also in the same ballpark, but a little higher. Why not glance at their densities?

Here we retrieve the posterior samples, compute the indirect effect, and summarize the indirect effect with quantile().

##   50%  2.5% 97.5% 
## 0.112 0.063 0.169

The results are similar to those in the text (p. 127). Here’s what it looks like.

Once again, those sweet Bayesian credible intervals get the job done.

Here’s a way to get both the direct effect, \(c'\) (i.e., b_withdraw_estress), and the total effect, \(c\) (i.e., \(c'\) + \(ab\)) of estress on withdraw.

## # A tibble: 2 x 4
##   name      mean     ll    ul
##   <chr>    <dbl>  <dbl> <dbl>
## 1 c        0.02  -0.089 0.129
## 2 c_prime -0.093 -0.195 0.013

Both appear pretty small. Which leads us to the next section…

4.3 Effect size

The quantification of effect size in mediation analysis is an evolving area of thought and research. [Hayes described] two measures of effect size that apply to the direct, indirect, and total effects in a mediation model…. For an excellent discussion of measures of effect size in mediation analysis, see Preacher and Kelley (2011). [We will] use their notation below. (p. 133)

4.3.1 The partially standardized effect.

We get \(SD\)s using the sd() function. Here’s the \(SD\) for our \(Y\) variable, withdraw.

## [1] 1.24687

Here we compute the partially standardized effect sizes for \(c'\) and \(ab\) by dividing those vectors in our post object by sd(estress$withdraw), which we saved as sd_y.

## # A tibble: 3 x 5
##   name         mean median     ll    ul
##   <chr>       <dbl>  <dbl>  <dbl> <dbl>
## 1 ab_ps       0.09   0.09   0.051 0.136
## 2 c_prime_ps -0.074 -0.074 -0.156 0.01 
## 3 c_ps        0.016  0.016 -0.072 0.104

The results are similar, though not identical, to those in the text. Here we have both rounding error and estimation differences at play. The plots:

On page 135, Hayes revisited the model from section 3.3. We’ll have to reload the data and refit that model to follow along. First, load the data.

Refit the model, this time with the bf() statements defined right within brm().

The partially-standardized parameters require some posterior_samples() wrangling.

## # A tibble: 3 x 5
##   name        mean median     ll    ul
##   <chr>      <dbl>  <dbl>  <dbl> <dbl>
## 1 ab_ps      0.157  0.151  0.006 0.339
## 2 c_prime_ps 0.163  0.166 -0.167 0.489
## 3 c_ps       0.32   0.322 -0.045 0.677

Happily, these results are closer to those in the text than with the previous example.

4.3.2 The completely standardized effect.

Note. Hayes could have made this clearer in the text, but the estress model he referred to in this section was the one from way back in section 3.5, not the one from earlier in this chapter.

One way to get a standardized solution is to standardize the variables in the data and then fit the model with those standardized variables. To do so, we’ll revisit our custom standardize(), put it to work, and fit the standardized version of the model from section 3.5, which we’ll call model4.4.

Fit the model.

Here they are, our newly standardized coefficients.

##                     Estimate Est.Error   Q2.5 Q97.5
## withdrawz_Intercept    0.000     0.056 -0.114 0.109
## affectz_Intercept     -0.001     0.060 -0.117 0.118
## withdrawz_estress_z   -0.089     0.060 -0.206 0.031
## withdrawz_affect_z     0.446     0.062  0.324 0.568
## affectz_estress_z      0.341     0.059  0.225 0.458

Here we do the wrangling necessary to spell out the standardized effects for \(ab\), \(c'\), and \(c\).

## # A tibble: 3 x 5
##   name        mean median     ll    ul
##   <chr>      <dbl>  <dbl>  <dbl> <dbl>
## 1 ab_s       0.152  0.15   0.093 0.223
## 2 c_prime_s -0.089 -0.09  -0.206 0.031
## 3 c_s        0.063  0.063 -0.06  0.185

Let’s confirm that we can recover these values by applying the formulas on page 135 to the unstandardized model, which we’ll call model5. First, we’ll have to fit that model since we haven’t fit that one since Chapter 3.

Check the unstandardized coefficient summaries.

##                    Estimate Est.Error   Q2.5 Q97.5
## withdraw_Intercept    1.448     0.253  0.960 1.936
## affect_Intercept      0.801     0.146  0.515 1.096
## withdraw_estress     -0.077     0.053 -0.181 0.026
## withdraw_affect       0.771     0.102  0.573 0.971
## affect_estress        0.172     0.030  0.113 0.231

On pages 135–136, Hayes provided the formulas to compute the standardized effects, which are

\[\begin{align*} c'_{cs} & = \frac{SD_X(c')}{SD_{Y}} = SD_{X}(c'_{ps}), \\ ab_{cs} & = \frac{SD_X(ab)}{SD_{Y}} = SD_{X}(ab_{ps}), \text{and} \\ c_{cs} & = \frac{SD_X(c)}{SD_{Y}} = c'_{cs} + ab_{ps}, \end{align*}\]

where the \(ps\) subscript indicates partially standardized. Here we put them in action to standardize the unstandardized results.

## # A tibble: 3 x 5
##   name        mean median     ll    ul
##   <chr>      <dbl>  <dbl>  <dbl> <dbl>
## 1 ab_s       0.152  0.151  0.091 0.221
## 2 c_prime_s -0.088 -0.088 -0.207 0.029
## 3 c_s        0.064  0.064 -0.059 0.188

Success!

4.3.3 Some (problematic) measures only for indirect effects.

Hayes recommended against these, so I’m not going to bother working any examples.

4.4 Statistical power

As Hayes discussed, power is an important but thorny issue within the frequentist paradigm. Given that we’re not particularly interested in rejecting the point-null hypothesis as Bayesians and that we bring in priors (which we’ve largely avoided explicitly mentioning in his project but have been quietly using all along), the issue is even more difficult for Bayesians. To learn more on the topic, check out Chapter 13 in Kruschke’s text, Miočević, MacKinnon, and Levy’s paper on power in small-sample Bayesian analyses or Gelman and Carlin’s paper offering an alternative to the power paradigm. You might look at Matti Vuorre’s Sample size planning with brms project. And finally, I have a series of blog posts on Bayesian power analyses. You can find the first post here.

4.5 Multiple \(X\)s or \(Y\)s: Analyze separately or simultaneously?

“Researchers sometimes propose that several causal agents (\(X\) variables simultaneously transmit their effects on the same outcome through the same mediator(s)” (p. 141).

4.5.1 Multiple \(X\) variables.

The danger in including multiple \(X\)’s in a mediation model, as when including statistical controls, is the possibility that highly correlated \(X\)s will cancel out each other’s effects. This is a standard concern in linear models involving correlated predictors. Two \(X\) variables (or an \(X\) variable and a control variable) highly correlated with each other may also both be correlated with \(M\) or \(Y\), so when they are both included as predictors of \(M\) or \(Y\) in a mediation model, they compete against each other in their attempt to explain variation in \(M\) and \(Y\). Their regression coefficients quantify their unique association with the model’s mediator and outcome variable(s). at the extreme, the two variables end up performing like two boxers in the ring simultaneously throwing a winning blow at the other at precisely the same time. Both get knocked out and neither goes away appearing worthy of a prize. The stronger the associations between the variables in the model, the greater the potential of such a problem. (pp. 143–144)

The same basic problems with multicollinearity applies to the Bayesian paradigm, too.

4.5.2 Estimation of a model with multiple \(X\) variables in PROCESS brms.

Hayes discussed the limitation that his PROCESS program may only handle a single \(X\) variable in the x= part of the command line, for which he displayed a workaround. We don’t have such a limitation in brms. Using Hayes’s hypothetical data syntax for a model with three \(X\)s, the brms code would be like this.

To show it in action, let’s simulate some data.

## # A tibble: 6 x 5
##      iv1    iv2     iv3    med     dv
##    <dbl>  <dbl>   <dbl>  <dbl>  <dbl>
## 1  0.217  0.177 -1.39   -0.755 -1.77 
## 2 -0.542  1.69   0.0513  0.721  0.402
## 3  0.891 -1.35   1.10    0.777 -0.132
## 4  0.596  1.08  -0.203  -0.955  1.02 
## 5  1.64  -0.456 -0.428  -2.89  -3.26 
## 6  0.689 -0.681 -0.429  -0.462 -2.38

Before we proceed, if data simulation is new to you, you might check out Roger Peng’s helpful tutorial or this great post by Ariel Muldoon.

Here we fit the model.

Behold the results.

##  Family: MV(gaussian, gaussian) 
##   Links: mu = identity; sigma = identity
##          mu = identity; sigma = identity 
## Formula: dv ~ 1 + iv1 + iv2 + iv3 + med 
##          med ~ 1 + iv1 + iv2 + iv3 
##    Data: d (Number of observations: 1000) 
## Samples: 4 chains, each with iter = 2000; warmup = 1000; thin = 1;
##          total post-warmup samples = 4000
## 
## Population-Level Effects: 
##               Estimate Est.Error l-95% CI u-95% CI Rhat Bulk_ESS Tail_ESS
## dv_Intercept     -0.01      0.03    -0.07     0.05 1.00     6381     2620
## med_Intercept     0.00      0.03    -0.06     0.06 1.00     8328     3209
## dv_iv1            0.02      0.05    -0.06     0.11 1.00     3143     2958
## dv_iv2            0.56      0.03     0.50     0.62 1.00     6816     3053
## dv_iv3            1.01      0.05     0.92     1.10 1.00     3072     2835
## dv_med            0.46      0.03     0.40     0.53 1.00     2598     3270
## med_iv1          -0.93      0.03    -0.99    -0.87 1.00     6508     2690
## med_iv2           0.03      0.03    -0.03     0.09 1.00     7471     2999
## med_iv3           0.98      0.03     0.92     1.04 1.00     6776     3352
## 
## Family Specific Parameters: 
##           Estimate Est.Error l-95% CI u-95% CI Rhat Bulk_ESS Tail_ESS
## sigma_dv      1.00      0.02     0.96     1.05 1.00     6757     3151
## sigma_med     0.97      0.02     0.93     1.02 1.00     7183     3073
## 
## Samples were drawn using sampling(NUTS). For each parameter, Eff.Sample 
## is a crude measure of effective sample size, and Rhat is the potential 
## scale reduction factor on split chains (at convergence, Rhat = 1).

Good old brms::brm() came through just fine. If you wanted to simulate data with a particular correlation structure for the iv variables, you might use the mvnorm() function from the MASS package, which you can learn more about here.

4.5.3 Multiple \(Y\) variables.

We’ve already been using the multivariate syntax in brms for our simple mediation models. Fitting a mediation model with multiple \(Y\) variables is a minor extension. To see, let’s simulate more data.

## # A tibble: 6 x 5
##       iv    med    dv1    dv2     dv3
##    <dbl>  <dbl>  <dbl>  <dbl>   <dbl>
## 1  0.217  0.285 -1.61   0.999  0.420 
## 2 -0.542  1.42   0.594  0.836  0.0208
## 3  0.891 -0.902  0.206  0.120 -0.954 
## 4  0.596  1.37  -0.799  0.530  3.13  
## 5  1.64   0.362 -2.06  -0.643  0.840 
## 6  0.689 -0.337 -1.12   0.487 -1.03

Fitting this requires a slew of bf() statements.

##  Family: MV(gaussian, gaussian, gaussian, gaussian) 
##   Links: mu = identity; sigma = identity
##          mu = identity; sigma = identity
##          mu = identity; sigma = identity
##          mu = identity; sigma = identity 
## Formula: dv1 ~ 1 + iv + med 
##          dv2 ~ 1 + iv + med 
##          dv3 ~ 1 + iv + med 
##          med ~ 1 + iv 
##    Data: d (Number of observations: 1000) 
## Samples: 4 chains, each with iter = 2000; warmup = 1000; thin = 1;
##          total post-warmup samples = 4000
## 
## Population-Level Effects: 
##               Estimate Est.Error l-95% CI u-95% CI Rhat Bulk_ESS Tail_ESS
## dv1_Intercept     0.01      0.03    -0.05     0.07 1.00     8052     3408
## dv2_Intercept     0.00      0.03    -0.06     0.06 1.00     9291     2822
## dv3_Intercept    -0.01      0.03    -0.07     0.05 1.00    11394     3264
## med_Intercept     0.03      0.03    -0.04     0.09 1.00     9076     3159
## dv1_iv           -1.05      0.04    -1.12    -0.98 1.00     7625     3621
## dv1_med           0.05      0.03    -0.01     0.11 1.00     6884     3490
## dv2_iv            0.05      0.04    -0.02     0.13 1.00     6359     3277
## dv2_med           0.53      0.03     0.47     0.59 1.00     7057     3574
## dv3_iv            1.03      0.04     0.96     1.10 1.00     6603     3204
## dv3_med           1.06      0.03     1.00     1.12 1.00     6709     3134
## med_iv            0.53      0.03     0.46     0.59 1.00     8371     2780
## 
## Family Specific Parameters: 
##           Estimate Est.Error l-95% CI u-95% CI Rhat Bulk_ESS Tail_ESS
## sigma_dv1     0.98      0.02     0.93     1.02 1.00     9101     3235
## sigma_dv2     0.97      0.02     0.93     1.02 1.00     8124     2685
## sigma_dv3     1.00      0.02     0.96     1.05 1.00     7786     2917
## sigma_med     1.00      0.02     0.96     1.04 1.00     9576     2698
## 
## Samples were drawn using sampling(NUTS). For each parameter, Eff.Sample 
## is a crude measure of effective sample size, and Rhat is the potential 
## scale reduction factor on split chains (at convergence, Rhat = 1).

Once again, brms to the rescue!

4.6 Chapter summary

Statistical mediation analysis has changed since the publication of Baron and Kenny (1986). The heyday of the causal steps “criteria to establish mediation” approach is over. Also disappearing in the 21 century is a concern about whether a process can be labeled as complete or partial mediation. Modern mediation analysis emphasizes an explicit estimation of the indirect effect, inferential tests of the indirect effect that don’t make unnecessary assumptions, and an acknowledgement that evidence of a statistically significant association between \(X\) and \(Y\) is not necessary to talk about a model intervening variable process (in which case the concepts of complete and partial mediation simply don’t make sense). (p. 146)

To this, I’ll just point out Hayes is speaking from a frequentist hypothesis-testing orientation. If you would like to dwell on significance tests, you certainty can. But particularly from within the Bayesian paradigm, you just don’t need to.

Session info

## R version 3.6.0 (2019-04-26)
## Platform: x86_64-apple-darwin15.6.0 (64-bit)
## Running under: macOS High Sierra 10.13.6
## 
## Matrix products: default
## BLAS:   /Library/Frameworks/R.framework/Versions/3.6/Resources/lib/libRblas.0.dylib
## LAPACK: /Library/Frameworks/R.framework/Versions/3.6/Resources/lib/libRlapack.dylib
## 
## locale:
## [1] en_US.UTF-8/en_US.UTF-8/en_US.UTF-8/C/en_US.UTF-8/en_US.UTF-8
## 
## attached base packages:
## [1] stats     graphics  grDevices utils     datasets  methods   base     
## 
## other attached packages:
##  [1] brms_2.10.3     Rcpp_1.0.2      forcats_0.4.0   stringr_1.4.0   dplyr_0.8.3     purrr_0.3.3    
##  [7] readr_1.3.1     tidyr_1.0.0     tibble_2.1.3    ggplot2_3.2.1   tidyverse_1.2.1
## 
## loaded via a namespace (and not attached):
##  [1] nlme_3.1-139         matrixStats_0.55.0   xts_0.11-2           lubridate_1.7.4      threejs_0.3.1       
##  [6] httr_1.4.0           rstan_2.19.2         tools_3.6.0          backports_1.1.5      utf8_1.1.4          
## [11] R6_2.4.0             DT_0.9               lazyeval_0.2.2       colorspace_1.4-1     withr_2.1.2         
## [16] prettyunits_1.0.2    processx_3.4.1       tidyselect_0.2.5     gridExtra_2.3        mnormt_1.5-5        
## [21] Brobdingnag_1.2-6    compiler_3.6.0       cli_1.1.0            rvest_0.3.4          shinyjs_1.0         
## [26] xml2_1.2.0           labeling_0.3         colourpicker_1.0     scales_1.0.0         dygraphs_1.1.1.6    
## [31] psych_1.8.12         callr_3.3.2          ggridges_0.5.1       StanHeaders_2.19.0   digest_0.6.21       
## [36] foreign_0.8-71       rmarkdown_1.13       base64enc_0.1-3      pkgconfig_2.0.3      htmltools_0.4.0     
## [41] htmlwidgets_1.5      rlang_0.4.1          readxl_1.3.1         rstudioapi_0.10      shiny_1.3.2         
## [46] generics_0.0.2       zoo_1.8-6            jsonlite_1.6         crosstalk_1.0.0      gtools_3.8.1        
## [51] inline_0.3.15        magrittr_1.5         loo_2.1.0            bayesplot_1.7.0      Matrix_1.2-17       
## [56] munsell_0.5.0        fansi_0.4.0          abind_1.4-5          lifecycle_0.1.0      stringi_1.4.3       
## [61] pkgbuild_1.0.5       plyr_1.8.4           grid_3.6.0           parallel_3.6.0       promises_1.1.0      
## [66] crayon_1.3.4         miniUI_0.1.1.1       lattice_0.20-38      haven_2.1.0          hms_0.4.2           
## [71] ps_1.3.0             zeallot_0.1.0        knitr_1.23           pillar_1.4.2         igraph_1.2.4.1      
## [76] markdown_1.1         shinystan_2.5.0      stats4_3.6.0         reshape2_1.4.3       rstantools_2.0.0    
## [81] glue_1.3.1.9000      evaluate_0.14        modelr_0.1.4         vctrs_0.2.0          httpuv_1.5.2        
## [86] cellranger_1.1.0     gtable_0.3.0         assertthat_0.2.1     xfun_0.10            mime_0.7            
## [91] xtable_1.8-4         broom_0.5.2          coda_0.19-3          later_1.0.0          viridisLite_0.3.0   
## [96] rsconnect_0.8.15     shinythemes_1.1.2    bridgesampling_0.7-2