Chapter 2 Meaning to Form

Written by Rinus Verdonschot
Edited by Vitória Piai for consistency and brevity

Have you ever wondered how people go from wanting to say something to actually speaking it out loud? This turns out to be quite complicated with numerous steps involved. However, we can all do this very quickly and efficiently as it typically takes only little over half a second before speaking starts (Indefrey and Levelt 2004) and we hardly make errors (Butterworth 1989).

Here, I will explain the basic steps of this ability and take the model of Levelt, Roelofs and Meyer (1999) as the main guide. However, you should be aware that there are other models which take different stances on some of the claims the Levelt et al. model makes (Bloem and La Heij 2003; Caramazza 1997; Dell 1986; Mahon et al. 2007). The model is laid out in the figure below to make it easier for you to follow the separate steps.

The Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer (1999) model of language production. (s= syllable, o= onset, n=nucleus, and c=coda)

Figure 2.1: The Levelt, Roelofs & Meyer (1999) model of language production. (s= syllable, o= onset, n=nucleus, and c=coda)

So, let’s start. Basically, there are two parts involved in speaking. The first part entails activating the meaning-related aspects of what one would like to say and the second part involves creating the sound form of the word. The Levelt et al. model assumes that some kind of network is involved here, with “nodes” at each level representing different kinds of information and “activation” spreading between these nodes.

2.1 Starting with the meaning

For example, if you see a big dog running towards you, you may want to shout “DOG” to your friends in order to make them aware of it. So, once the concept “DOG” becomes activated, it will spread some of its activation to its neighbours. In other words, if you want to say “DOG,” actually semantically related concepts like “CAT” or “HORSE” also become available, especially if they are closer together in the network.

2.2 The lexical level

Any activated concept will spread some of its activation to its so-called “lemma” node. This is a memory representation of the word’s meaning and its syntactic properties. With syntactic properties we mean that languages may store particular syntactic information of words here. For example, whether it is countable (e.g., “three dogs”) or uncountable (e.g., “food,” but not “three foods”), or if a language has a gender system (like German, French, Dutch, etc.), the gender of the word would be stored at this level. So, in German, for example, the lemma node for dog (“Hund,” [der Hund]) would have “masculine” and “countable” assigned to it (amongst other things).

Assume for now that only the DOG and CAT concepts are activated and they both send activation to their respective lemma nodes. According to the Levelt et al. (1999) model, only for one of them the sound form can be constructed, that is: one lemma must be selected for production. But how could a lemma win then? One way it could win is by assuming there are inhibitory links between activated lemmas. In other words, the “dog” lemma would be activated and inhibitory links would exist to other activated lemmas (e.g., cat) which it can then use to actively extinguish them. However, the Levelt et al. model does not adopt any inhibitory links anywhere. Instead, it assumes that the activation of nontarget lemmas affects the time it takes to select the target lemma. In other words, lemmas like cat and dog do compete for selection over time (but not via inhibitory links). To decide who wins, the model uses a simple mathematical rule (a so-called “hazard rate”) to calculate the probability an activated lemma “survives” or “dies” over time depending on the activation of all co-activated lemmas. Once a lemma is selected (i.e., “survives”), activation will cross from the conceptual/syntactic information (Part 1 in Figure 2.1) to the phonological/articulation level (Part 2 in Figure 2.1).

That this division (i.e., between Part 1 and 2) is not a simple trivial thing can be seen for example in the “tip-of-the-tongue” phenomenon (see Section 1) in which one would know some semantic information (e.g., “Hmm. That actor who plays in Avatar”) or whether something is countable or not, but one could not (completely) reach the pronunciation or only part of it (e.g., “S…Sa.. something”).

2.3 The phonological level

Once a lemma has been selected for production, it will activate a so-called “lexeme.” Now, three pieces of information become available for the word you would like to produce. First, the morphology of a word. In the case of < dog > this is simple as it is a monomorphemic word (i.e., you cannot break down < dog > into smaller units that still carry meaning). But a word such as < reappear > consists of two morphemes: < re- > and < appear >. Another case would be a plural, so if your intention was to say “DOGS,” then the lemma dog would have become activated and a syntactic “flag” for plurality would have been set there, then at the lexeme level both < dog > and < s > (plural suffix of dog) would become activated.

Next, the so-called metrical information becomes available, which (for Dutch, English, and German for example) states how many syllables the word consists of and where the stress is located. In the case of < dog > this is easy as it is just one stressed syllable (σ’; σ = syllable, ‘ = stress). But, for example, the metrical information for < forest > would specify that it has two syllables (σ’σ) with stress on the first syllable (i.e., FOrest).

Concurrently, the individual segments of the word would become available, that is: /d/, /ɔ/, /g/ (the ɔ means an open-mid back rounded vowel). Then a process would start called “prosodification” in which the individual segments would be incrementally put into the metrical frame. Meaning that /d/, /ɔ/, /g/ would be incrementally put into σ’ resulting in [dɔg]. Huh? You might say. Why so complicated, why not just store [dɔg]? Well, for “dog” it is easy, but many languages have things such as re-syllabification. Re-syllabification means that depending on the word that precedes or follows a word, the “original” syllable may change. For example, if you take the word combination “hike is” in “This hike is fun,” then you will notice that “hike is” is syllabified as [haɪ] [kiz] so the /k/ did not stay with /hike/ but moved to the next word /is/. It is impossible to store all possible word combinations, so it is very likely that a process such as prosodification exists, though perhaps not in the same form for all languages (Verdonschot, Han, and Kinoshita 2021).

So, what we finally end up with is a representation that will be used to guide the creation of a gestural score (that is, the motor plan to move the articulators to produce speech). How this works will be dealt with in Section 3.

Take-home messages

  • There are two parts involved in speaking: the activation of meaning-related aspects of the intended words and the creation of the sound form of the words
  • From an activated concept, activation will spread to a “lemma” node (a memory representation of the word’s meaning and its syntactic properties)
  • From a selected lemma, the phonological information is activated, containing information over morphemes, syllables and stress patterns, and individual sounds (phonemes)

Suggestions for further reading
The interested reader is referred to additional literature on models of word production, including the theoretical debates in the field (Dell and O’Seaghdha 1992; Rahman and Melinger 2019; Caramazza 1997; Mahon et al. 2007; Roelofs, Piai, and Schriefers 2013).

Exercise 2.1 Think about the errors you collected in section 1. Can you work out at what stage (from those discussed above) they emerged? Note that it could be that they emerge during another stage that we haven’t discussed yet.

References

Bloem, I., and W. La Heij. 2003. “Semantic Facilitation and Semantic Interference in Word Translation: Implications for Models of Lexical Access in Language Production.” Journal of Memory and Language 48 (3): 468–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00503-X.
Butterworth, Brian. 1989. Lexical Access in Speech Production.” In W. Marslen-Wilson (Ed.), Lexical Representation and Process, 108–35. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Caramazza, Alfonso. 1997. “How Many Levels of Processing Are There in Lexical Access?” Cognitive Neuropsychology 14 (1): 177–208. https://doi.org/10.1080/026432997381664.
Dell, Gary S. 1986. “A Spreading-Activation Theory of Retrieval in Sentence Production.” Psychological Review 93 (3): 283–321. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283.
Dell, Gary S., and Padraig G. O’Seaghdha. 1992. “Stages of Lexical Access in Language Production.” Cognition 42 (1-3): 287–314. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90046-K.
Indefrey, Peter, and W. J. M. Levelt. 2004. “The Spatial and Temporal Signatures of Word Production Components.” Cognition 92 (1-2): 101–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001.
Levelt, W. J. M., Ardi Roelofs, and Antje S. Meyer. 1999. “A Theory of Lexical Access in Speech Production.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (1): 1–38. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99001776.
Mahon, Bradford Z., Albert Costa, Robin Peterson, Kimberly A. Vargas, and Alfonso Caramazza. 2007. “Lexical Selection Is Not by Competition: A Reinterpretation of Semantic Interference and Facilitation Effects in the Picture-Word Interference Paradigm.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33 (3): 503–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.503.
Rahman, Rasha Abdel, and Alissa Melinger. 2019. “Semantic Processing During Language Production: An Update of the Swinging Lexical Network.” Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 34 (9): 1176–92. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1599970.
Roelofs, Ardi, Vitória Piai, and Herbert Schriefers. 2013. “Context Effects and Selective Attention in Picture Naming and Word Reading: Competition Versus Response Exclusion.” Language and Cognitive Processes 28 (5): 655–71. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.615663.
Verdonschot, Rinus G, Jeong-Im Han, and Sachiko Kinoshita. 2021. “The Proximate Unit in Korean Speech Production: Phoneme or Syllable?” Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 74 (1): 187–98. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021820950239.