2.4 Carry-over effect and washout periods

In the Himalaya study, what if patients spent two weeks on the Himalaya 292 diet, then the next two weeks on the refined cereal diet?

Potentially, the influence of the first diet could still be impacting the subjects' faecal weight for a little while after stopping the first diet. This could compromise the internal validity of the study.

This is called the carryover effect.

Definition 2.2 (Carryover effect) The carry-over effect is when the influence of past experience(s) of the individuals carry over to influence future experience(s) of the individuals.

In the context of experiments, this may mean that the influence of one treatment carries over into the influence of the next treatment.

Sometimes, researchers can randomly allocate the order in which the treatments (i.e., the diets) are used. That is, some participants start by spending four weeks on the Himalaya 292 diet, then (after a washout period) four weeks on the refined cereal diet; meanwhile, other participants start by spending four weeks on the refined cereal diet, then (after a washout period) four weeks on the Himalaya 292 diet.

Example 2.8 (Washout periods) A study of paramedics (MacDonald et al. 2006) required paramedics to conduct eight different tasks (such as electrical defibrillation and intravenous cannulation).

The order in which each of the 16 paramedics performed the eight tasks was arranged so that not every paramedic started with Task 1, followed by Task 2, etc. to "control for possible effects of practice" (p.  255); that is, to mitigate the carry-over effect.

The impact of the carryover effect also can be minimized by using a washout period or similar; for example, after finishing one diet, the participants spend four weeks on their usual (before study) diet, and then revert to the second diet being used.

Example 2.9 (Carry-over effect) In the Himalaya 292 study, the authors report:

Subjects were allocated randomly to [...] dietary treatments according to a cross-over study design with each intervention phase lasting 4 weeks. There was no washout period between phases.

--- Bird et al. (2008), p. 1033

That is, subjects were randomly allocated to a diet: some subjects began the study on the Himalaya 292 diet while others started on the refined cereal diet. No washout period was used; however, since the response variable was recorded after four weeks on the diets, no washout period was necessary.

Example 2.10 (Washout periods) An engineering study (Miller and Boyle 2019) examined drivers' exposure to lane-keeping system on their driving performance. Subjects were exposed to a driving simulation that used a lane-keeping system, and then to a driving simulation without using a lane-keeping system.

The researchers found that there was a carryover effect when drivers moved from a simulation with a lane-keeping system to one without a lane-keeping system.
Using a 'washout' period to minimize the carry-over effect

Figure 2.5: Using a 'washout' period to minimize the carry-over effect

References

Bird, Anthony R., Michelle S. Vuaran, Roger A. King, Manny Noakes, Jennifer Keogh, Matthew K. Morell, and David L. Topping. 2008. “Wholegrain Foods Made from a Novel High-Amylose Barley Variety (Himalaya 292) Improve Indices of Bowel Health in Human Subjects.” British Journal of Nutrition 99: 1032–40.
MacDonald, Russell D., Vicki LeBlanc, Brad McArthur, and Adam Dubrowski. 2006. “Performance of Resuscitation Skills by Paramedic Personnel in Chemical Protective Suits.” Prehospital Emergency Care 10 (2): 254–59.
Miller, Erika E., and Linda Ng Boyle. 2019. “Behavioral Adaptations to Lane Keeping Systems: Effects of Exposure and Withdrawal.” Human Factors 61 (1): 152–64.